社交技能在12月龄儿童运动和理解性语言之间的中介作用
收稿日期: 2023-08-07
网络出版日期: 2024-05-10
基金资助
贵州省卫生健康委科学技术基金项目(gzwkj2022-199);贵阳市科技计划基金资助项目(筑科合同[2020]-16-16号)
Mediating role of social skills between motor skills and comprehensible language in 12-month-old children
Received date: 2023-08-07
Online published: 2024-05-10
目的 探究12月龄儿童运动与理解性语言之间的关系,并考察社交技能在二者之间的中介作用。方法 选择出生队列中2022年1月—2023年5月在我院儿童保健门诊进行定期随访的12月龄儿童作为研究对象,采用调查问卷收集一般人口学信息,使用年龄发育进程问卷-第三版(ASQ-3)评估儿童的运动和社交技能,采用汉语沟通发展量表-普通话版(PCDI)评估儿童的理解性语言。结果 在12月龄儿童中,大运动、精细运动、社交与理解性语言均呈正相关;调整协变量后,社交技能在12月龄儿童的大运动[β=0.27,95%CI(0.17~0.39)]、精细运动[β=0.45,95%CI(0.30~0.62)]和理解性语言之间起完全中介作用。结论 在12月龄儿童中,运动可以通过社交技能间接促进理解性语言的发展。
简兰英 , 骆艳 , 朱焱 , 沈成威 , 涂可心 , 张琼 , 梅馨洁 , 覃会玲 , 金骊芳 . 社交技能在12月龄儿童运动和理解性语言之间的中介作用[J]. 临床儿科杂志, 2024 , 42(5) : 434 -438 . DOI: 10.12372/jcp.2024.23e0754
Objective To explore the relationship between motor skills and comprehensible language development, and to examine the mediating effects of social skills on them in 12-month-old children. Methods Twelve-month-old infants in the birth cohort who underwent regular follow-up at the Child Health Clinic of Guiyang Maternal and Child Health Hospital from January 2022 to May 2023 were used as the study subjects. A questionnaire survey was performed to collect demographic information. Ages and Stages Questionnaire-Third Edition (ASQ-3) was used to evaluate infants’ motor and social skills. Comprehensible language was evaluated by using Chinese Communicative Development Inventory-Putonghua version (PCDI). Results In 12-month-old children,gross motor, fine motor and social skills were positively correlated with comprehensible language. After adjustment for covariates, social skill played a complete mediating role between gross motor [β=0.27, 95%CI(0.17-0.39)], fine motor [β=0.45, 95%CI(0.30-0.62)] and comprehensible language development in 12-month-old children. Conclusion In 12-month-old children, motor skills can indirectly contribute to the development of comprehensible language through social skills.
Key words: gross motor; fine motor; social skill; comprehensible language; mediation effect
[1] | Gonzalez SL, Alvarez V, Nelson EL. Do gross and fine motor skills differentially contribute to language outcomes? A systematic review[J]. Front Psychol, 2019, 10: 2670. |
[2] | Walle EA, Campos JJ. Infant language development is related to the acquisition of walking[J]. Dev Psychol, 2014, 50(2): 336-348. |
[3] | 陈玲, 王艾东, 谷长玉. 如何提高听障儿童生活情景中的理解性语言[J]. 中国听力语言康复科学杂志, 2021(z1): 55-56. |
[4] | Deserno MK, Fuhrmann D, Begeer S, et al. Longitudinal development of language and fine motor skills is correlated, but not coupled, in a childhood atypical cohort[J]. Autism, 2023, 27(1): 133-144. |
[5] | 毛萌, 江帆. 儿童保健学[M]. 第4版. 北京: 人民卫生出版社, 2020: 47-53. |
[6] | Cheung WC, Meadan H, Xia Y. A longitudinal analysis of the relationships between social, communication, and motor skills among students with autism[J]. J Autism Dev Disord, 2021, 52(10): 4505-4518. |
[7] | Hockema S A, Smith L B. Learning your language, outside-in and inside-out[J]. Linguistics, 2009, 47(2): 435-479. |
[8] | Squires J, Bricker D. 年龄与发育进程问卷使用指南[M]. 第3版. 卞晓燕,译. 上海: 上海科学技术出版社, 2009: 15. |
[9] | Tardif T. 汉语沟通发展量表使用手册[M]. 北京: 北京大学医学出版社, 2008: 70-71. |
[10] | 温忠麟, 张雷, 侯杰泰, 等. 中介效应检验程序及其应用[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(5): 614-620. |
[11] | Libertus K, Violi DA. Sit to talk: relation between motor skills and language development in infancy[J]. Front Psychol, 2016, 7: 475. |
[12] | Smith LB. It's all connected: Pathways in visual object recognition and early noun learning[J]. Am Psychol, 2013, 68(8): 618-629. |
[13] | Lebarton ES, Iverson JM. Fine motor skill predicts expressive language in infant siblings of children with autism[J]. Dev Sci, 2013, 16(6): 815-827. |
[14] | 段小倩, 石慧峰, 窦妍, 等. 亲子互动对0-3岁留守儿童语言发育的影响[J]. 中国生育健康杂志, 2020, 31(5): 420-425. |
[15] | Ohara R, Kanejima Y, Kitamura M, et al. Association between social skills and motor skills in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review[J]. Eur J Investig Health Psychol Educ, 2019, 10(1): 276-296. |
[16] | 张颖, 王俊峰, 丁艳华. 儿童语言发育迟缓的临床特征和影响因素研究[J]. 中国儿童保健杂志, 2022, 30(8): 912-915. |
[17] | 曹璟钰, 王利刚, 陶婷, 等. 母亲受教育程度与学前儿童语言早期发展的meta分析[J]. 中国心理卫生杂志, 2021, 35(12): 1019-1024. |
[18] | 牛杰, 陈永香, 朱莉琪. 早产儿语言发展的影响因素[J]. 中国当代儿科杂志, 2012, 14(1): 76-80. |
[19] | Alcock KJ, Krawczyk K. Individual differences in language development: relationship with motor skill at 21 months[J]. Dev Sci, 2010, 13(5): 677-691. |
[20] | Oudgenoeg-Paz O, Volman MC, Leseman PP. Attainment of sitting and walking predicts development of productive vocabulary between ages 16 and 28 months[J]. Infant Behav Dev, 2012, 35(4): 733-736. |
[21] | Morales MF, Farkas C, Aristotelous E, et al. The impact of contextual, maternal and prenatal factors on receptive language in a chilean longitudinal birth cohort[J]. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev, 2021, 52(6): 1106-1117. |
[22] | Cattani A, Bonifacio S, Fertz M, et al. Communicative and linguistic development in preterm children: a longitudinal study from 12 to 24 months[J]. Int J Lang Commun Disord, 2010, 45(2): 162-173. |
[23] | Stillman CM, Cohen J, Lehman ME, et al. Mediators of physical activity on neurocognitive function: a review at multiple levels of analysis[J]. Front Hum Neurosci, 2016, 10: 626. |
[24] | Koba? M, K?z?ldere E, Do?an I, et al. Motor skills, language development, and visual processing in preterm and full-term infants[J]. Current Psychology, 2022, 42(15): 12463-12475. |
[25] | Smith L, Gasser M. The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies[J]. Artif Life, 2005, 11(1-2): 13-29. |
[26] | Hellendoorn A, Wijnroks L, Van Daalen E, et al. Motor functioning, exploration, visuospatial cognition and language development in preschool children with autism[J]. Res Dev Disabil, 2015, 39: 32-42. |
[27] | Houwen S, Visser L, Van Der Putten A, et al. The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities[J]. Res Dev Disabil, 2016, 53-54: 19-31. |
/
〈 |
|
〉 |